Search over 40,000 articles from the original, classic Encyclopedia Britannica, 11th Edition.
See also: COLOURS OF ANIMALS . Much See also:interest attaches in See also:modern See also:biology to the questions involved in the colours of animals. The subject may best be considered in two divisions: (1) as regards the uses of See also:colour in the struggle for existence and in sexual relationships; (2) as regards the chemical See also:causation. 1. BIo oaiics Use of Colour for Concealment.—Cryptic colouring is by far the commonest use of colour in the struggle for existence. It is employed for the purpose of attack (aggressive resemblance or anticryptic colouring) as well as of See also:defence (protective resemblance or procryptic colouring). The fact that the same method, concealment, may be used both for attack and defence has been well explained by T. See also:Belt (The Naturalist in See also:Nicaragua, See also:London, 1888), who suggests as an See also:illustration the rapidity of See also:movement which is also made use of by both pursuer and pursued, which is similarly raised to a maximum in both by the See also:gradual dying out of the slowest through a See also:series of generations. Cryptic colouring is commonly associated with other See also:aids in the struggle for See also:life. Thus well-concealed mammals and birds, when discovered, will generally endeavour to See also:escape by See also:speed, and will often See also:attempt to defend themselves actively. On the other See also:hand, small animals which have no means of active defence, such as large See also:numbers of See also:insects, frequently depend upon concealment alone. Protective resemblance is far commoner among animals than aggressive resemblance, in See also:correspondence with the fact that predaceous forms are as a See also:rule much larger and much less numerous than their See also:prey.In the See also: case of insectivorous Vertebrate and their prey such See also:differences exist in an exaggerated See also:form. Cryptic colouring, whether used for defence or attack, may be either See also:general or See also:special. In general resemblance the See also:animal, in consequence of its colouring, produces the same effect as its environment, but the conditions do not require any special See also:adaptation of shape and outline. General resemblance is especially See also:common among the animals inhabiting some uniformly coloured expanse of the See also:earth's See also:surface, such as an ocean or a See also:desert. In the former, animals of all shapes are frequently protected by their transparent See also:blue colour; on the latter, equally diverse forms are defended by their sandy See also:appearance. The effect of a See also:uniform appearance may be produced by a See also:combination of tints in startling contrast. Thus the See also:black and See also:
In all cases the resemblance is to some See also: object which is of no interest to the enemy or prey respectively. The animal is not hidden from view by becoming indistinguishable from its background, as in the cases of general resemblance, but it is mistaken for some well-known object. In seeking the See also:interpretation of these most interesting and elaborate adaptations, attempts have been made along two lines. First, it is sought to explain the effect as a result of the See also:direct See also:influence of the environment upon the individual (G. L. L. See also:Buffon), or by the inherited effects of effort and the use and disuse of parts (J. $. P. See also:Lamarck). Second, natural selection is believed to have produced the result, and afterwards maintained it by the survival of the best concealed in each See also:generation. The former suggestions break down when the complex nature of numerous special resemblances is appreciated.Thus the arrangement of colours of many kinds into an appropriate See also: pattern requires the co-operation of a suitable shape and the rigidly exact See also:adoption of a certain elaborate attitude. The latter is instinctive, and thus depends on the central See also:nervous See also:system. The cryptic effect is due to the exact co-operation of all these factors; and in the See also:present See also:state of See also:science the only possible See also:hope of an interpretation lies in the theory of natural selection, which can accumulate any and every variation which tends towards survival. A few of the See also:chief types of methods by which concealment is effected may be briefly described. The colours of large numbers of Vertebrate animals are darkest on the back, and become gradually lighter on the sides, passing into white on the belly. See also:Abbott H. See also:Thayer (The See also:Auk, vol. xiii., 1896) has suggested that this gradation obliterates the appearance of solidity, which is due to See also:shadow. The colour-See also:harmony, which is also essential to concealment, is produced because the back is of the same tint as the environment (e.g. earth) bathed in the See also:cold blue-white of the See also:sky, while the belly, being cold blue-white bathed in shadow and yellow earth reflections, produces the same effect. Thayer has made See also:models (in the natural See also:history museums at London, See also:Oxford and See also:Cambridge) which support his interpretation in a very convincing manner. This method of neutralizing shadow for the purpose of concealment by increased lightness of tint was first suggested by E. B. Poulton in the case of a larva (Trans.Ent. See also: Soc. Lond., 1887, p. 294) and a pupa (Trans. Ent. Soc. Lond., 1888, pp. 596, 597) but he did not appreciate the See also:great importance of the principle. In an analogous method an animal in front of a background of dark shadow may have part of its See also:body obliterated by the existence of a dark tint, the See also:remainder resembling, e.g., a part of a leaf (W. See also:
W. Spengel, See also: Jena, 1886). This method of rendering invisible any part which would interfere with the resemblance is well known in See also:mimicry. A common aid to concealment is the adoption by different individuals of two or more different appearances, each of which resembles some special object to which an enemy is indifferent. Thus the leaf-like butterflies (Kallima) present various types of colour and pattern on the under See also:side of the wings, each of which closely resembles some well-known appearance presented by a dead leaf; and the common See also:British yellow under-wing See also:moth (Tryphaena pronuba) is similarly polymorphic on the upper side of its upper wings, which are exposed as it suddenly drops among dead leaves. Caterpillars and pupae are also commonly dimorphic, See also:green and See also:
If sedentary, like the former example, they are covered up with See also: local materials; if wandering, like the latter, they have the instinct to reclothe. Allocryptic methods may also be used for aggressive purposes, as the See also:ant-See also:lion larva, almost buried in See also:sand, or the large See also:frog Ceratophrys, which covers its back with earth when waiting for its prey. Another form of allocryptic defence is found in the use of the colour of the See also:food in the See also:digestive See also:organs showing through the transparent body, and in certain cases the adventitious colour may be dissolved in the See also:blood or secreted in superficial cells of the body: thus certain insects make use of the See also:chlorophyll of their food (Poulton, Proc. See also:Roy. Soc. liv. 417). The most perfect cryptic See also:powers are possessed by those animals in which the individuals can change their colours into any tint which would be appropriate to a normal environment. This See also:power is widely prevalent in See also:fish, and also occurs in See also:Amphibia and Reptilia (the See also:chameleon affording a well-known example). Analogous powers exist in certain See also:Crustacea and See also:Cephalopoda. All these rapid changes of colour are due to changes in shape or position of superficial pigment cells controlled by the nervous system. That the latter is itself stimulated by See also:light through the See also:medium of the See also:eye and optic See also:nerve has been proved in many cases. Animals with a See also:short life-history passed in a single environment, which, however, may be very different in the case of different individuals, may have a different form of variable cryptic colouring, namely, the power of adapting their colour once for all (many pupae), or once or twice (many larvae).In these cases the effect appears to be produced through. the nervous system, although the stimulus of light probably acts on the skin and not through the eyes. Particoloured surfaces do not produce particoloured pupae, probably because the antagonistic stimuli neutralize each other in the central nervous system, which then disposes the superficial colours so that a neutral or intermediate effect is produced over the whole surface (Poulton, Trans. Ent. Soc. Lond., 1892, p. 293). Cryptic colouring may incidentally produce superficial resemblances between animals; thus desert forms concealed in the same way may gain a likeness to each other, and in the same way special resemblances, e.g. to lichen, bark, See also: grasses, See also:pine-needles, &c., may sometimes See also:lead to a tolerably See also:close similarity between the animals which are thus concealed. Such likeness may be called syncryptic or common protective (or aggressive) resemblance, and it is to be distinguished from mimicry and common warning colours, in which the likeness is not incidental, but an end in itself. Syncryptic resemblances have much in common with those incidentally caused by functional adaptation, such as the See also:mole-like forms produced in the burrowing Insectivora, See also:Rodentia and See also:Marsupialia. Such likeness may be called syntechnic resemblance, incidentally produced by dynamic similarity, just as syncryptic resemblance is produced by static similarity. Use of Colour for Warning and Signalling, or Sematic Coloration. —The use of colour for the purpose of warning is the exact opposite of the one which has been just described, its object being to render the animal conspicuous to its enemies, so that it can be easily seen, well remembered, and avoided in future.Warning colours are associated with some quality or weapon which renders the possessor unpleasant or dangerous, such as unpalatability, an evil odour, a sting, the See also: poison-See also:fang, &c. The object being to warn an enemy off, these colours are also called aposematic. Recognition markings, on the other hand, are episematic, assisting the individuals of the same species to keep together when their safety depends upon numbers, or easily to follow each other to a See also:place of safety, the See also:young and inexperienced benefiting by the example of the older. Episematic characters are far less common than aposematic, and these than cryptic; although, as regards the latter comparison, the opposite impression is generally produced from the very fact that concealment. is so successfully attained. Warning or aposematic colours, together with the qualities they indicate, depend, as a rule, for their very existence upon the abundance of palatable food supplied by the animals with cryptic colouring. Unpalatability, or even the See also:possession of a sting, is not sufficient defence unless there is enough food of another See also:kind to be obtained at the same See also:time and place (Poulton, Proc. Zool. Soc., 1887, p. 191). Hence insects with warning colours are not seen in temperate countries except at the time when See also:insect life as a whole is most abundant; and in warmer countries, with well-marked wet and dry seasons, it will probably be found that warning colours are proportionately less See also:developed in the latter. In many species of See also:African butterflies belonging to the genus Junonia (including Precis) the wet-See also:season broods are distinguished by the more or less conspicuous under sides of the wings, those of the dry season being highly cryptic. Warning colours are, like cryptic, assisted by special adaptations of the body-form, and especially by movements which assist to render the colour as conspicuous, as possible.On this See also: account animals with warning colours generally move or See also:fly slowly, and it is the rule in butterflies that the warning patterns are similar on both upper and under sides of the wings. Many animals, when attacked or disturbed, " sham See also:death " (as it is commonly but wrongly described), falling motionless to the ground. In the case of well-concealed animals this instinct gives them a second See also:chance of escape in the earth or among the leaves, &c., when they have been once detected; animals with warning colours are, on the other hand, enabled to assume a position in which their characters are displayed to the full (J. Portschinsky, Lepidopterorum Rossiae Biologia, St See also:Petersburg, 189o, See also:plate i. See also:figs. 16, 17). In both cases a definite attitude is assumed, which is not that of death. Other warning characters exist in addition to colouring: thus See also:sound is made use of by the disturbed rattle-snake and the See also:Indian Echis, &c. Large birds, when attacked, often adopt a threatening attitude, accompanied by a terrifying sound. The See also:cobra warns an intruder chiefly by attitude and the dilation of the flattened See also:neck, the effect being heightened in some species by the " See also:spectacles." In such cases we often see the combination of cryptic and sematic methods, the animal being concealed until disturbed, when it instantly assumes an aposematic attitude. The See also:advantage to the animal itself is clear: a poisonous snake gains nothing by killing an animal it cannot eat; while the poison does not cause immediate death, and the enemy would have time to injure or destroy the snake. In the case of small unpalatable animals with warning colours the enemies would only first become aware of the unpleasant quality by tasting and often destroying their prey; but the species would gain by the experience thus conveyed, even though the individual might suffer. An insect-eating animal does not come into the See also:world with knowledge: it has to be educated by experience, and warning colours enable this See also:education as to what to avoid to be gained by a small instead of a large See also:waste of life.Further-more, great tenacity of life is usually possessed by animals with warning colours. The tissues of aposematic insects generally possess great See also: elasticity and power of resistance, so that large numbers of individuals can recover after very severe treatment. The brilliant warning colours of many caterpillars attracted the See also:attention of See also:Darwin when he was thinking over his hypo-thesis of sexual selection, and he wrote to A. R. See also:Wallace on the subject (C. Darwin, Life and Letters, London, 1887, iii. 93). Wallace, in reply, suggested their interpretation as warning colours, a See also:suggestion since verified by experiment (Proc. Ent. Soc. Lond., 1867, p. Ixxx; Trans.Ent. Soc. Lond., 1869, pp. 21 and 27). Although animals with warning colours are probably but little attacked by the See also: ordinary enemies of their class, they have special enemies which keep the numbers down to the See also:average. Thus the See also:cuckoo appears to be an insectivorous See also:bird which will freely devour conspicuously coloured unpalatable larvae. The effect of the warning colours of caterpillars is often intensified by gregarious habits. Another aposematic use of colours and structures is to divert attention from the vital parts, and thus give the animal attacked an extra chance of escape. The large, conspicuous, easily torn wings of butterflies and moths See also:act in this way, as is found by the abundance of individuals which may be captured with notches bitten symmetrically out of both wings when they were in contact. The eye-spots and " tails " so common on the hinder part of the See also:hind wing, and the conspicuous See also:apex so frequently seen on the fore wing, probably have this meaning. Their position corresponds to the parts which are most often found to be notched. In some cases (e.g. many Lycaenidae) the " tail " and eye-spot combine to suggest the appearance of a See also:head with antennae at the posterior end of the butterfly, the deception being aided by movements of the hind wings.The See also: flat-topped " tussocks " of See also:hair on many caterpillars look like conspicuous fleshy projections of the body, and they are held prominently when the larva is attacked. If seized, the " tussock " comes out, and the enemy is greatly inconvenienced by the See also:fine branched hairs. The tails of lizards, which easily break off, are to be similarly explained, the attention of the pursuer being probably still further diverted by the extremely active movements of the amputated member. Certain crabs similarly throw off their claws when attacked, and the claws continue to snap most actively. The tail of the See also:dormouse, which easily comes off, and the extremely bushy tail of the See also:squirrel, are probably of use in the same manner. Animals with warning colours often tend to resemble each other superficially. This fact was first pointed out by H. W. See also:Bates in his See also:paper on the theory of mimicry (Trans. Linn. Soc. vol. See also:xxiii., 1862, p. 495).He showed that the conspicuous, presumably unpalatable, tropical See also: American butterflies, belonging to very different See also:groups, which are mimicked by others, also tend to resemble each other, the likeness being often remark-ably exact. These resemblances were not explained by his theory of mimicry, and he could only suppose that they had been produced by the direct influence of a common environment. The problem was solved in 1879 by Fritz Muller (see Proc. Ent. Soc. Lond., 1879, p. xx.), who suggested that life is saved by this resemblance between warning colours, inasmuch as the education of young inexperienced enemies is facilitated. Each species which falls into a See also:group with common warning (synaposematic) colours contributes to See also:save the lives of the other members. It is sufficiently obvious that the amount of learning and remembering, and consequently of injury and loss of life involved in the See also:process, are reduced when many species in one place possess the same aposematic colouring, instead of each exhibiting a different " danger-See also:signal." These resemblances are often described as " Mullerian mimicry," as distinguished from true or " Batesian mimicry " described in the next See also:section. Similar synaposematic resemblances between the specially protected groups of butterflies were afterwards shown to exist in tropical See also:Asia, the See also:East Indian Islands and See also:Polynesia by F. See also:Moore (Proc. Zool. Soc., 1883, p.201), and in Africa by E. B. Poulton (See also: Report Brit. Assoc., 1897, p. 688). R. Meldola (See also:Ann. and Mag. Nat. Hist. R., 1882, p. 417) first pointed out and explained in the same manner the remarkable general uniformity of colour and pattern which runs through so many species of each of the distasteful groups of butterflies; while, still later, Poulton (Proc. Zool.Soc., 1887, p. 191) similarly extended the interpretation to the synaposematic resemblances between animals of all kinds in the same See also: country. Thus, for example, See also:longitudinal or circular bands of the same strongly contrasted colours are found in species of many groups with distant See also:affinities. Certain animals, especially the Crustacea, make use of the special defence and warning colours of other animals. Thusthe See also:English See also:hermit-crab, Pagurus Bernhardus, commonly carries the sea-See also:anemone, Sagartia parasitica, on its See also:shell; while another English species, Pagurus Prideauxii, inhabits a shell which is invariably clothed by the flattened Adamsia palliata. The white patch near the tail which is frequently seen in the gregarious Ungulates, and is often rendered conspicuous by adjacent black markings, probably assists the individuals in keeping together; and appearances with probably the same interpretation are found in many birds. The white upturned tail of the See also:rabbit is probably of use in enabling the individuals to follow each other readily. The difference between a typical aposematic See also:character appealing to enemies, and episematic intended for other individuals of the same species, is well seen when we compare such examples as (1) the huge banner-like white tail, conspicuously contrasted with the black or black and white body, by which the slow-moving See also:skunk warns enemies of its power of emitting an intolerably offensive odour; (2) the small upturned white tail of the rabbit, only seen when it is likely to be of use and when the owner is moving, and, if pursued, very rapidly moving, towards safety. Mimicry (see also MIMICRY) or Pseudo-sematic Colours.--The fact that animals with distant affinities may more or less closely resemble each other was observed See also:long before the existing ex-planation was possible. Its recognition is implied in a number of insect names with the termination formis, usually given to species of various orders which more or less closely resemble the stinging See also:Hymenoptera. The usefulness of the resemblance was suggested in See also:Kirby and See also:Spence's Introduction to See also:Entomology, London, 1817, H. 223.H. W. Bates (Trans. Linn. Soc. vol. xxiii., 1862, p. 495) first proposed an explanation of mimicry based on the theory of natural selection. He supposed that every step in the formation and gradual improvement of the likeness occurred in consequence of its usefulness in the struggle for life. The subject is of additional interest, inasmuch as it was one of the first attempts to apply the theory of natural selection to a large class of phenomena up to that time well known but unexplained. Numerous examples . of mimicry among tropical American butterflies were discussed by Bates in his paper; and in 1866 A. R. Wallace extended the See also: hypothesis to the butterflies of the tropical East (Trans. Linn, Soc. vol. See also:xxv., 1866, p.19); See also: Roland Trimen (Trans. Linn. Soc. vol. See also:xxvi., 1870, p. 497) to those of Africa in 187o. The See also:term mimicry is used in various senses. It is often extended, as indeed it was by Bates, to include all the superficial resemblances between animals and any part of their environment. Wallace, however, separated the cryptic resemblances already described, and the See also:majority of naturalists have followed this convenient arrangement. In cryptic resemblance an animal resembles some object of no interest to its enemy (or prey), and in so doing is concealed; in mimicry an animal resembles some other animal which is specially disliked by its enemy, or some object which is specially attractive to its prey, and in so doing becomes conspicuous. Some naturalists have considered mimicry to include all superficial likenesses between animals, but such a See also:classification would group together resemblances which have widely different uses. (1) The resemblance of a mollusc to the See also:coral on which it lives, or an external See also:parasite to the hair or skin of its See also:host, would be procryptic; (2) that between moths which resemble lichen, syncryptic; (3) between distasteful insects, synaposematic; (4) between the Insectivor mole and the Rodent mole-See also:rat, syntechnic; (5) the essential element in mimicry is that it is a false warning (pseud-aposematic) or false recognition (pseudepisematic) character. Some have considered that mimicry indicates resemblance to a moving object; but apart from the non-mimetic likenesses between animals classified above, there are ordinary cryptic resemblances to drifting leaves, swaying bits of twig, &c., while truly mimetic resemblances are often specially adapted for the attitude of See also:rest. Many use the term mimicry to include synaposematic as well as pseudo-sematic resemblances, calling the former " Mullerian," the latter " Batesian," mimicry.The objection to this grouping is that it takes little account of the deceptive element which is essential in mimicry. In synaposematic colouring the warning is genuine, in pseudaposematic it is a sham. The term mimicry has led to much misunderstanding from the fact that in ordinary speech it implies deliberate See also: imitation. The See also:production of mimicry in an individual animal has no more to do with consciousness or "taking thought" than any of the other processes of growth. Protective mimicry is here defined as an advantageous and superficial resemblance of one animal to another, which latter is specially defended so as to be disliked or feared by the majority of enemies of the groups to which both belong—a resemblance which appeals to the sense of sight, sometimes to that of See also:hearing, and rarely to See also:smell, but does not extend to deep-seated characters except when the superficial likeness is affected by them. Mutatis mutandis this See also:definition will apply to aggressive (pseudepisematic) resemblance. The conditions under which mimicry occurs have been stated by Wallace:—" (1) that the imitative species occur in the same area and occupy the same station as the imitated; (2) that the imitators are always the more defence-less; (3) that the imitators are always less numerous in individuals; (4) that the imitators differ from the bulk of their See also:allies; (5) that the imitation, however See also:minute, is external and visible only, never extending to See also:internal characters or to such as do not affect the external appearance." It is obvious that conditions 2 and 3 do not hold in the case of Miillerian mimicry. Mimicry has been explained, independently of natural selection, by the supposition that it is the common expression of the direct See also:action of common causes, such as See also:climate, food, &c.; also by the supposition of See also:independent lines of See also:evolution leading to the same result without any selective action in consequence of advantage in the struggle; also by the operation of sexual selection. It is proposed, in conclusion, to give an account of the broad aspects of mimicry, and attempt a brief discussion of the theories of origin of each class of facts (see Poulton, Linn. Soc. Journ. Zool., 1898, p.558)• It will be found that in many cases the See also: argument here made use of applies equally to the origin of cryptic and sematic colours. The relationship between these classes has been explained: mimicry is, as Wallace has stated (Darwinism, London, 1889), merely " an exceptional form of protective resemblance." Now, protective (cryptic) resemblance cannot be explained on any of the lines suggested above, except natural selection; even sexual selection fails, because cryptic resemblance is especially common in the immature stages of insect life. But it would be unreasonable to explain mimetic resemblance by one set of principles and cryptic by another and totally different set. Again, it may be plausible to explain the mimicry of one butterfly for another on one of the suggested lines, but the resemblance of a fly or moth to a See also:wasp is by no means so easy, and here selection would be generally conceded; yet the See also:appeal to antagonistic principles to explain such closely related cases would only be justified by much direct See also:evidence. Furthermore, the mimetic resemblances between butterflies are not haphazard, but the models almost invariably belong only to certain sub-families, the Danainae and Acraeinae in all the warmer parts of the world, and, in tropical See also:America, the Ithomiinae and Heliconinae as well. These groups have the characteristics of aposematic species, and no theory but natural selection explains their invariable occurrence as models wherever they exist. It is impossible to suggest, except by natural selection, any explanation of the fact that mimetic resemblances are confined to changes which produce or strengthen a superficial likeness. Very deep-seated changes are generally involved, inasmuch as the appropriate instincts as to attitude, &c., are as important as colour and marking. The same conclusion is reached when we analyse the nature of mimetic resemblance and realize how complex it really is, being made up of colours, both pigmentary and structural, pattern, form, attitude and movement. A plausible interpretation of colour may be wildly improbable when applied to some other element, and there is no explanation except natural selection which can explain all these elements. The appeal to the direct action of local conditions in common often breaks down upon the slightest investi-gation, the difference in habits between mimic and See also:model in the same locality causing the most complete divergence in their conditions of life. Thus many insects produced from burrowing larvae mimic those whose larvae live in the open.Mimetic resemblance is far commoner in the See also: female than in the male, a fact readily explicable by selection, as suggested by Wallace, for the female is compelled to fly more slowly and to expose itself while laying eggs, and hence a resemblance to the slow-flying freely exposed models is especially advantageous. The facts that mimetic species occur in the same locality, fly at the same time of the year as their models, and are See also:day-flying species even though they may belong to nocturnal groups, are also more or less difficult to explain except on the theory of natural selection, and so also is the fact that mimetic resemblance is produced in the most varied manner. A spider resembles its model, an ant, by a modification of its body-form into a superficial resemblance, and by holding one pair of legs to represent antennae; certain bugs (See also:Hemiptera) and beetles have also gained a shape unusual in their respective groups, a shape which superficially resembles an ant; a Locustid (Myrmecophana) has the shape of an ant painted, as it were, on its body, all other parts resembling the background and invisible; a Membracid (Homoptera) is entirely unlike an ant, but is concealed by an ant-like See also:shield. When we further realize that in this and other examples of mimicry " the likeness is almost always detailed and remarkable, however it is attained, while the methods differ absolutely," we recognize that natural -selection is the only possible explanation hitherto suggested. In the cases of aggressive mimicry an animal resembles some object which is attractive to its prey. Examples are found in the See also:flower-like species of See also:Mantis, which attract the insects on which they feed. Such cases are generally described as possessing " alluring colours," and are regarded as examples of aggressive (anticryptic) resemblance, but their logical position is here. Colours displayed in Courtship, Secondary Sexual Characters, Epigamic Colours.—Darwin suggested the explanation of these appearances in his theory of sexual selection (The Descent of See also:Man, London, 1874). The rivalry of the See also:males for the possession of the See also:females he believed to be decided by the preference of the latter for those individuals with especially See also:bright colours, highly developed plumes, beautiful See also:song, &c. Wallace does not accept the theory, but believes that natural selection, either directly or indirectly, accounts for all the facts. Probably the majority of naturalists follow Darwin in this respect. The subject is most difficult, and the interpretation of a great proportion of the examples in a high degree uncertain, so that a very brief account is here expedient.That selection of some kind has been operative is indicated by the diversity of the elements into which the effects can be analysed. The most complete set of observations on epigamic display was made by See also:
See also: Cunningham has argued (Sexual Dimorphism in the Animal See also:Kingdom, London, 1900) that secondary sexual characters have been produced by direct stimulation due to contests, &c., in the breeding See also:period, and have gradually become hereditary, a hypothesis involving the assumption that acquired characters are transmitted. Wallace suggests that they are in part to be explained as " recognition characters," in part as an indication of surplus vital activity in the male. 2. See also:CHEMISTRY The coloration of the surface of animals is caused either by See also:pigments, or by a certain structure of the surface by means of which the light falling on it, or reflected through its superficial trans-See also:parent layers, undergoes diffraction or other See also:optical change. Or it may be the result of a combination of these two causes. It plays an important part in the relationship of the animal to its environment, in concealment, in mimicry, and so on; the presence of a pigment in the integument may also serve a more direct physiological purpose, such as a See also:respiratory See also:function. The coloration of birds' feathers, of the skin of many fishes, of many insects, is partially at least due to structure and the action of the See also:peculiar pigmented cells known as " chromatophores " (which W. Garstang defines as pigmented cells specialized for the See also:discharge of the See also:chromatic function), and is much better marked when these have for their background a " reflecting layer " such as is provided by guanin, a substance closely related to uric See also:acid. Such a mechanism is seen to greatest advantage in fishes. Among these, guanin may be present in a finely granular form, causing the light falling on it to be scattered, thus producing a white effect; or it may be present in a peculiar crystalline form, the crystals being known as " iridocytes "; or in a layer of closely apposed needles forming a silvery See also:sheet or See also:mirror. In the See also:iris of some fishes the See also:golden red colour is produced by the light reflected from such a layer of guanin needles having to pass through a thin layer of a reddish pigment, known as a " lipochrome." Again, in some lepidopterous insects a white or a yellow appearance is produced by the deposition of uric acid or a nearly allied substance on the surface of the wings. In many animals, but especially among invertebrates, colouring matters or pigments play an important role in surface coloration; in some cases such coloration may be of benefit to the animal, but in others the integument simply serves as an See also:organ for the See also:excretion of waste pigmentary substances.Pigments (1) may be of direct physiological importance; (2) they may be excretory; or (3) they may be introduced into the body of the animal with the food. Of the many pigments which have been described up to the present time, very few have been subjected to elementary chemical See also: analysis, owing to the great difficulties attending their See also:isolation. An extremely small amount of pigment will give rise to a great amount of coloration, and the pigments are generally accompanied by impurities of various kinds which cling to them with great tenacity, so that when one has been thoroughly cleansed very little of it remains for ultimate analysis. Most of these substances have been detected by means of the spectroscope, their absorption bands serving for their recognition, but See also:mere identity of spectrum does not necessarily mean chemical identity, and a few chemical tests have also to be applied before a conclusion can be See also:drawn. The absorption bands are referred to certain definite parts of the spectrum, such as the See also:Fraunhofer lines, or they may be given in See also:wave-lengths. For this purpose the readings of the spectroscope are reduced to wave-lengths by means of See also:interpolation curves; or if Zeiss's microspectroscope be used, the position of bands in wave-lengths (denoted by the See also:Greek See also:letter X) may be read directly. Haemoglobin, the red colouring See also:matter of vertebrate73s blood, C758H12o3NIe5S3FeOn8, and its derivatives haematin, Ca2HaoN4FeOa, and haematoporphyrin, C16H18N20s, are colouring matters about which we possess definite chemical knowledge, as they have been isolated, purified and analysed. Most of the bile pigments of mammals have likewise been isolated and studied chemically, and all of these are fully described in the See also:text-books of See also:physiology and physiological chemistry. Haemoglobin, though physiologically of great importance in the respiratory process of vertebrate animals, is yet seldom used for surface pigmentation, except in the See also:face of white races of man or in other parts in monkeys, &c. In some worms the transparent skin allows the haemoglobin of the blood to be seen through the integument, and in certain fishes also the haemoglobin is visible through the integument. It is a curious and noteworthy fact that in some invertebrate animals in which no haemoglobin occurs, we meet with its derivatives. Thus haematin is found in the so-called bile of slugs, snails, the limpet and the See also:crayfish.In sea-anemones there is a pigment which yields some of the decomposition-products of haemoglobin, and associated with this is a green pigment apparently identical with biliverdin (C16H18N204), a green bile pigment. Again, haematoporphyrin is found in the integuments of See also: star-fishes and slugs, and occurs in the " dorsal streak " of the earth-See also:worm Lumbricus terrestris, and perhaps in other species. Haematoporphyrin and biliverdin also occur in the See also:egg-shells of certain birds, but in this case they are derived from haemoglobin. Haemoglobin is said to be found as See also:low down in the animal kingdom as the Echinoderms, e.g. in Ophiactis virens and Thyonella gemmata. It also occurs in the blood of Planorbis corneus and in the pharyngeal muscles of other See also:mollusca. A great number of other pigments have been described; for example, in the muscles and tissues of animals, both vertebrate and invertebrate, are' the histohaematins, of which a special muscle pigment, myohaematin, is one. In vertebrates the latter is generally accompanied by haemoglobin, but in invertebrates—with the exception of the pharyngeal muscles of the molluscait occurs alone. Although closely related to haemoglobin or its derivative haemochromogen, the histohaematins are yet totally distinct, and they are found in animals where not a trace of haemoglobin can be detected. Another interesting pigment is turacin, which contains about 7% of See also:nitrogen, found by See also:Professor A. H. See also:
From it may be obtained turacoporphyrin, which is identical with haematoporphyrin, and gives the See also: band in the ultra-See also:violet which J. L. Soret and subsequently A. Gamgee have found to be characteristic of haemoglobin and its compounds. Turacin itself gives a peculiar two-banded spectrum, and contains about 7 % of See also:copper in its See also:molecule. Another copper-containing pigment is haemocyanin, which in the oxidized state gives a blue colour to the blood of various Mollusca and See also:Arthropoda. Like haemoglobin, it acts as an See also:oxygen-See also:carrier in respiration, but it takes no part in surface coloration. . A class of pigments widely distributed among See also:plants and animals are the lipochronles. As their name denotes, they are allied to See also:fat and generally accompany it, being soluble in fat solvents. They play an important part in surface coloration, and may be greenish, yellow or red in colour. They contain no nitrogen. As an example of a lipochrome which has been isolated, crystallized and purified, we may mention carotin, which has recently been found in green leaves.Chlorophyll, which is so often associated with a lipochrome, has been found in some See also: Infusoria, and in See also:Hydra and Spongilla, &c. In some cases it is probably formed by the animal; in other cases it may be due to symbiotic algae, while in the gastric gland of many Mollusca, Crustacea and Echinodermata it is derived from food-chlorophyll. Here it is known as entero-chlorophyll. The black pigments which occur among both vertebrate and invertebrate animals often have only one attribute in common, viz. blackness, for among the discordant results of analysis one thing is certain, viz. that the melanins from vertebrate animals are not identical with those from invertebrate animals. The melanosis or blackening of insect blood, for instance, is due to the oxidation of a chromogen, the pigment produced being known as a uranidine. In some See also:sponges a somewhat similar pigment has been noticed. Other pigments have been described, such as actiniochrome, echinochrome, pentacrinin, antedonin, polyperythrin (which appears to be a haematoporphyrin), the floridines, spongioporphyrin, &c., which need no mention here; all these pigments can only be distinguished by means of the spectroscope. Most of the pigments are preceded by colourless substances known as " chromogens," which by the action of the oxygen of the See also:air and by other agencies become changed into the corresponding pigments. In some cases the pigments are built up in the tissues of an animal, in others they appear to be derived more or less directly from the food. Derivatives of chlorophyll and lipochromes especially, seem to be taken up from the See also:intestine, probably by the agency of leucocytes, in which they may occur in combination with, or dissolved by, fatty matters and excreted by the integument. In worms especially, the skin seems to excrete many effete substances, pigments included. No direct connexion has been traced between the chlorophyll eaten with the food and the haemoglobin of blood and muscle.Attention may, however, be drawn to the See also: work of Dr E. Schunck, who has shown that a substance closely resembling haematoporphyrin can be prepared from chlorophyll; this is known as phylloporphyrin. Not only does the visible spectrum of this substance resemble that of haematoporphyrin, but the invisible ultra-violet also, as shown by C. A. Schunck. The reader may refer to E. A. Schafer's Text-See also:Book of Physiology (1898) for A. Gamgee's See also:article " On Haemoglobin, and its Compounds "; to the writer's papers in the Phil. Trans. and Proc. Roy. Soc. from 1881 onwards, and also Quart.Journ. Micros. Science and Journ. of Physiol. ; to C. F. W. Krukenberg's Vergleichende physiologische Studien from 1879 onwards, and to his Vortrdge. See also: Miss M. I. Newbigin collected in Colour in Nature (1898) most of the See also:recent literature of this subject. Dr E. Schunck's papers will be found under the heading " Contribution to the Chemistry of Chlorophyll " in Proc.Roy. Soc. from 1885 onwards; and Mr C. A. Schunck's paper in Proc. Roy. Soc. vol. lxiii. (C. A. Additional information and CommentsThere are no comments yet for this article.
» Add information or comments to this article.
Please link directly to this article:
Highlight the code below, right click, and select "copy." Then paste it into your website, email, or other HTML. Site content, images, and layout Copyright © 2006 - Net Industries, worldwide. |
|
[back] COLOUR (Lat. color, connected with celare, to hide,... |
[next] COLOURS, MILITARY |